Bullying: A Tendency of Peer
Victimization at Schools
Bullying is
defined as repeated oppression, physical or mental, of a less powerful person
by a more powerful person or group of persons. It occurs where there is an
imbalance in power between people, and it is a persistent or continued
unwelcome behaviuor. It is a kind of behaviour characterized by
intentionality and hurtfulness. Negative actions can be physical contact (such
as hitting or kicking), words (e.g., teasing, and calling names), or obscene
gestures or facial expressions. Negative actions also include the intentional
exclusion of a student and the spreading of rumours.
There is no universally agreed-upon
definition of bullying; however, Dan Olweus, who is considered the "pioneer
in bullying research" (Dake, 2003, p. 173) has defined
bullying, or peer victimization, in a way that has been generally well-received
and widely used. According to Olweus (1994), bullying occurs when a student is
"exposed, repeatedly over time, to negative actions on the part of one or
more other students" (Annotation:Bullying
at School:basic Facts and Effects of a School Based Intervention Program,
1994, p. 1173) .
A negative action is when someone either attempts to, or intentionally
inflicts, discomfort or injury on another student. This can be achieved through
several means. Negative actions also include the intentional exclusion of a
student and the spreading of rumours. In order for behaviour to be described as
bullying, there must be an imbalance in power "an asymmetrical power
relationship" (Olweus 1174) so that
the student exposed to the negative action has difficulty defending him or
herself, and is somewhat helpless against the harassment. This way, "bullying
cannot apply to a conflict between students of equal physical, mental, or
emotional strength" (Dake, 2003, p. 175) . Similarly, “Bullying
is a form of peer abuse that includes acts of aggression in which one or more
students physically and/or psychologically harass a weaker victim" (Hoover,
1992, p. 14) .
The cause of bullying is only the imbalance of power. It has both physical and
psychological effects upon victim.
Bullying is defined in the literature as
“a subset of aggressive behaviour, comprising a physical, verbal, or
psychological attack by one or more individuals" (Swain 361). In The
nature of school bullying: A cross-national perspective, Olweus clearly
defines “bullying and violence (or violent behaviour) as two subcategories of aggression" (The Nature of School Bullying: A
Cross-national Perspective, 1999, p. 9) . Bullying causes
violence. Bothe the bullying and violence are aggressive behavior.
With the increasing public
concern about school safety, researchers have broadened the definition of
school violence to include “any conditions or acts that create a climate in
which individual students and teachers feel fear or intimidation in addition to
being the victims of assault, theft, or vandalism" (Batsche, 1994, p. 165) . For Batsche,
bullying creates a fearful climate to students and teachers. Similarly, R.J.
Hazler in his article “Bullying breeds violence. You can stop it.” views that bullying
in school has devastating effects on students, often leading to “violent and
disastrous consequences for both victims and bullies" (Bullying breeds violence. You can stop it!, 1994, p.
39) .
P.T. Slee says that victims
of bullying suffer from “a loss of self-esteem lasting long into their adult
life" (Situational
and interpersonal correlates of anxiety associated with peer victimization,
1994, p. 99) .
Bullying does not effect for the timing. It creates long term effect upon
victims. Victims of bullying suffer from a loss of self-esteem into their adult
life. According to Farrington bullies in
school are very likely to bully their spouses and children later. He argues
that “society is the utmost victim of bullying because bullies in school are
very likely to bully their spouses and children later, which perpetuates the
cycle of domestic violence and creates new generations of aggressive children" (p. 21) . Thus, bullying has
long term effect. It creates cycle of domestic violence which can be seen even
in new generations.
Bullying and victimization among youth
have received increased attention in recent years by researchers and educators
internationally. Researchers have found that these phenomena are widespread
among school-age children across several countries in Europe (Timmerman, 2003) , North America (Pellegrini A. D., 2002) , and Oceania (Smith, 2000) . However, we are
unaware of any published studies on either bullying or victimization of
school-age children that have been conducted in Nepal. Thus, the purpose of the
current study was to examine bullying, victimization and self-esteem among
school-age children in Kathmandu, Nepal.
1.2 Correlates of Bullying and Victimization
Gender predicts bullying, victimization
and self-esteem. “Boys in Europe and the United States report more bullying as
perpetrators and as victims than do girls" (Almeida,
1999, p. 185) .
“Generally, boys engage in physical and direct bullying, such as pushing,
shoving, or kicking, whereas girls engage in verbal and indirect bullying, such
as intentional exclusion from the group, spreading rumors, teasing, or name
calling" (Almeida 178). According to Lane, “[b]ullying for boys is more
likely to be part of power-based social relationships and for girls affiliation
activities are more frequently the source of bullying activities" (p. 213) . Such differences
seem to originate in early gender socialization. The literature on physical
play activity suggests that boys “engage much more often in rough-and-tumble
play than girls do in virtually all cultures studied" (Pellegrini & Smith, 1998, p.
578) .
Pellegrini and Smith suggested that rough-and-tumble
play provides the means “in which boys assess the strength of others for dominance
purposes" (589).
In the United States, Bosworth et al.
examined the correlates of bullying in a sample of middle school students. They
found, as expected, that “boys engaged in higher amounts of bullying than girls
did" (1999, p. 352) . Similarly, research
in Australia conducted by Rigby and Slee (1991) found that "of 685 school
children (6-16 years old), 16.8% of boys and 11.4% of girls reported being
bullied very often" (621). These findings make it apparent that bullying
is widespread in schools. In Nepal, there is currently no reliable data on the
prevalence of bullying among school students. This study is therefore a
pioneering attempt to investigate the prevalence of the problem view to
sensitizing Nepalese researchers to the need for more research in this area. Different
researchers have categorized bullying into different types. Björkqvist and Niemelä suggest that "bullying can be
categorized into the dichotomies physical versus verbal and direct versus
indirect" (14). On another much related research bullying is categorized
into three types "direct physical, direct verbal and indirect
aggression" (Direct and
Indirect Aggression Scales (DIAS), 1992, p. 16) . Physical bullying
includes such direct behaviors as pushing another, hitting, punching, or
kicking. Verbal bullying may take the form of yelling abuse at another,
name-calling, using insulting expressions, or make verbal threats. On the other
hand, indirect bullying refers to the behavior such as spreading malicious
rumors about another, excluding a person from the group, or disclosing
another's secrets to a third person.
Similarly, I. Rivers and P.K. Smith in
their book Aggressive Behaviour opine
about the types of bullying that:
direct and
physical (e.g., hitting, pushing, kicking, and damaging property); direct and
verbal (e.g., name calling, teasing, being laughed at, and threatening); or
indirect (e.g., spreading nasty rumours, purposefully excluding an individual
from a social group, sending nasty/rude text messages or emails, and placing
information about the victim on the internet) (362)
Direct forms of
bullying can be defined as relatively open attacks on a victim that are carried
out face to face. Indirect forms of bullying, as being more subtle and less
direct and will include behavior such as social isolation and exclusion from a
group. Taking all of this into account, the
present study used the physical, verbal and indirect bullying definitions of
Björkqvist, Niemelä et al. (1992) as stated above, particularly as these
constructs have been "widely used in aggression and bullying research in
many countries" (Sex
differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review, 2004,
p. 298) .
Having briefly outlined the different forms of aggression and bullying, the
discussion will now turn to the associations between gender, age and
aggression.
In explaining
sex differences in social behaviour, Eagly (1987) proposed a social role
theory, whereby "people behave in a manner that is consistent with
their gender roles" (Sex differences in social behavior: A social role
interpretation, 1987, p. 28) . These roles have
arisen from social divisions relating to domestic and work-related roles, such
that females primarily carry out domestic and child rearing duties and are more
likely to fill positions in the workplace that are communal in nature (e.g.,
nurse, teacher). Through experiencing and enacting gender roles, males and
females develop different skills, attitudes, and expectancies resulting in behavior
patterns that differ according to those gender-roles. Consequently, there are "normative
expectations that males are more agentic (instrumental, masculine) and females
are more communal (expressive, feminine), with these gender norms passed on
through socialization processes to future generations" (Eagly & Wood, 1991, p. 317) .
There are
numerous studies that have found " males to be more physically and
verbally aggressive than females" (Bettencourt & Miller, 1996, p. 435) . For example, Owens
and MacMullin (1995) used a peer-estimation method with 422 students from
grades 2, 6, 9, and 11 (mean ages 7.9, 11.9, 14.7, & 16.6 years,
respectively). Results from this study showed that boys were estimated by their
peers to show significantly more physical and verbal aggression (against other
boys) than girls (against girls) in all year levels. The one exception was in grade
9, with boys and girls not differing significantly in the degree of verbal
aggression. Taking the above findings into account and social role theory, the
present study predicted that boys would report significantly higher levels of
physical and verbal aggression than girls.
Consequently, it
is necessary to review a variety of studies specifically relating to gender
differences in indirect aggression in children and young people. There are
numerous studies showing that adolescent girls typically exhibit more indirect
aggressive behavior than boys. In a large cross-cultural study of aggression in
8-, 11-, and 15-year-old children (N = 2,094), peer-estimations (participants
estimate the extent to which peers behave in certain ways) showed that between
41% and 55% of girls’ aggressive behaviors were indirect, whereas the proportion
for boys ranged between 20% and 26% (Österman
et al., 1998). The proportions of verbal aggression for girls varied between
31% and 40% (boys 37- 47%), and from 8% to 20% for physical aggression (boys
33-37%). Other studies using peer-nomination (students name peers who display
certain behaviors) and peer-ratings (students estimate the frequency that named
classmates perform aggressive acts) have also found that girls exhibit more
indirect aggressive behavior than boys (e.g., Björkqvist, 1994; Lagerspetz et
al., 1988). The above findings correspond with Australian research which used a
peer-estimation technique whereby adolescent participants were asked to
estimate how often fellow students in their class behaved in specific ways to
others (girls to girls and boys to boys). These studies found that girls used
significantly more indirect aggression than boys in grades 9 and 11 (Owens,
1996; Owens & MacMullin, 1995). In similar and more recent Australian
research described above (Owens et al., 2005), 591 adolescents from Years 8
through 10 provided self-report levels of indirect victimization, with results
showing that, whereas boys reported more physical and verbal victimization than
girls, girls experienced significantly higher levels of overall indirect
victimization.
Having discussed gender differences
in aggression, it now remains to consider the connection between age and
aggression. It is difficult to remove gender from the relationship as
development trajectories differ as a function of gender, as do social roles and
stereotypes (e.g., Cairns et al., 1989; Eagly & Steffen, 1986). To
illustrate, Archer asserts that, "although social role theory (Eagly,
1987) makes no specific predictions regarding gender, age, and aggression,
social learning theory (Bandura, 1973) provides some clarification"(296).
Archer states that the two theories parallel each other such that social learning
processes (e.g., observation, modeling) facilitate the acquisition and maintenance
of aggressive behaviors in accordance with social roles. Effectively, social
learning predicts that "gender differences will initially be minor in
childhood and will increase with age due to the cumulative impact of socialization
processes" (Archer 297).
Although we are unaware of any published
studies on either bullying or victimization of school-age children that have
been conducted in Nepal, a researcher Niti Rana from Kathmandu University has raised
issue in her dissertation. It seems that research on bullying and victimization
among school age children is needed. The current study is part of a large
investigation concerning bullying and victimization among school-age children
in Nepal.
Works
Cited
Almeida, A. (1999). The nature of school
bullying: Across-national perspective. (P. M.-T. Smith, Ed.) London:
Routledge.
Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in
real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology,
8, 291-322.
Batsche, G. &. (1994). Bullies and their
victims:Understanding a pervasive problem in the schools. School
Psychology Review, 23, 165-174.
Bettencourt, B., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender
differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 119, 422-447.
Björkqvist, K. &. (1992). Of mice and
woman:Aspect of female aggression. In K. &. Björkqvist, New trends in
the study of female aggression (pp. 3-16). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Björkqvist, K. L. (1992). Direct and Indirect
Aggression Scales (DIAS). Vasa, Finland: Department of Social Sciences,
Abo Akademi University.
Bosworth, K., Espelage, D., & Simon, T. (1999).
Factors associated wih bullying behavior in middle school students. Journal
of Early Adolescence, 19, 341-362.
Charach, A., & Pepler, D. &. (1995).
Bullying at school: A Canadian perspective. Education Canada, 35,
12-18.
Dake, J. J. (2003). The Nature and Extent of
Bullying at School. Journal of School Health, 171-179.
Eagly, A. (1987). Sex differences in social
behavior: A social role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Eagly, A., & Wood, W. (1991). Explaining sex
differences in social behavior: A meta-analytic perspective. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 306-315.
Farrington, D. (1991). Childhood aggression and
adult violence: Early precursors and later-life outcomes. (D. &. Pepler,
Ed.) The development and treatment of children aggression, 5-29.
Hazler, R. (1994). Bullying breeds violence. You can
stop it! Learning, 22(6), 38-41.
Hoover, J. ,. (1992). Bullying: Perceptions of
sdoloscent victims in the midwestern U.S.A. School Psychology
International, 13, 5-16.
Lane, D. (1989). Bullying in school: The need for an
integrated approach. School Psychology International, 10, 211-215.
Olweus. (1999). The Nature of School Bullying: A
Cross-national Perspective. London: Routledge.
Olweus, D. (1994). Annotation:Bullying at
School:basic Facts and Effects of a School Based Intervention Program. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1171-1190.
Osterman, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K.,
Kaukiainen, A., Landau, S., & Fraczek, A. e. (1998). Cross-cultural
evidence of female aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 1-8.
Owens, L. (1996). Sticks and stones and sugar and
spice: Girls' and boys' aggression in schools. Australian Journal of
Guidance and Counselling,6, 45-55.
Owens, L., & MacMullin, C. (1995). Gender
differences in aggression in children and adolescents in South Australian
schools. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 6, 21-35.
Owens, L., Shute, R., & Slee, P. (2005). In the
eye of the beholder... Girls', boys' and teachers' perceptions of boys'
aggression to girls. International Education Journal, 142-151.
Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). A longitudinal study of
bullying, dominance, and victimization during transition from primary school
through secondary school. British Journal of Development Psychology, 20,
259-280.
Pellegrini, A., & Smith, P. (1998). Physical
activity play: The nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child
Development, 69, 577-598.
Rigby, K. &. (1991). Bullying among Australian school
children: Reported behavior and attitudes toward victims. Journal of
Social Psychology, 131, 615-627.
Rivers, I. &. (1994). Types of bullying
behaviour and their correlates. Aggressive Behaviour, 359-368.
Slee, P. (1994). Situational and interpersonal
correlates of anxiety associated with peer victimization. Child Psychology
and Human Development, 25, 97-107.
Smith, P. &. (2000). Bullying in schools:
Lessons from two decades of research. Agressive Behavior, 26, 1-9.
Swain, J. (1998). What does bullying really mean? Educational
Research, 358-364.
Timmerman, G. (2003). Sexual harassment of
adolescents perpetrated by teachers and by peers: An exploration of the
dynamics of power, culture, and gender in secondary schools. Sex Roles, 48,
231-244.
Underwood, M., Galen, B., & Paquette, J. (2001).
Top ten challenges for understanding gender and aggression in children: Why
can't we all just get along? Social Development, 10, 248-266.
I really appreciate DR AKHIGBE,my name is LAURIE HUGHES . I will never stop testifying DR AKHIGBE , Happiness is all i see now I never thought that I will be cured from HIV virus again. DR AKHIGBE did it for me I have been suffering from a deadly disease (HIV) for the past 2 years now, I had spent a lot of money going from one place to another, from churches to churches, hospitals have been my home every day residence. Constant checks up have been my hobby not until this faithful day, I saw a testimony on how DR AKHIGBE helped someone in curing his HIV disease in internet quickly I copied his email which is drrealakhigbe@gmail.com just to give him a test I spoke to him, he asked me to do some certain things which I did, he told me that he is going to provide the herbal cure to me, which he did, then he asked me to go for medical checkup after some days, after using the herbal cure and i did, behold I was free from the deadly disease,till now no HIV in me again he only asked me to post the testimony through the whole world, faithfully am doing it now,all the testimony of DR AKHIGBE is true please BROTHER and SISTER, MOTHER and FATHER he is great, I owe him in return. if you are having a similar problem just email him on drrealakhigbe@gmail.com or you can whats App his mobile number on +2349010754824 He can also cure these diseases like HIV and AIDS HERPES,DIABETICS,CANCER, HEPATITIS A&B,CHRONIC DISEASES, HIGH BLOOD PRESURE, ASTHMA, HEART DISEASES, EXTERNAL INFECTION, EPILEPSY, STROKE, MULTIPLE SCLEROSIS, NAUSEA,VOMITING OR DIARRHEA LUPUS,ECZEMA,BACK PAIN,JOINT PAIN. .ETC .please email drrealakhigbe@gmail.com or whats APP him ..+2349010754824 he is a real good and honest man.
ReplyDeletewebsite... https:drrealakhigbe.weebly.com